Chapter 10 – STrengthening the REporting of Genetic Association studies (STREGA)—an extension of the STROBE statement
‹View Table of Contents
Human Genome Epidemiology (2nd ed.): Building the evidence for using genetic information to improve health and prevent disease
necessarily represent the views of the funding agency.”
These chapters were published with modifications by Oxford University Press (2010)
Julian Little, Julian P. T. Higgins, John P. A. Ioannidis, David Moher, France Gagnon, Erik von Elm, Muin J. Khoury, Barbara Cohen, George Davey Smith, Jeremy Grimshaw, Paul Scheet, Marta Gwinn, Robin E. Williamson, Guang Yong Zou, Kimberley Hutchings, Candice Y. Johnson, Valerie Tait, Miriam Wiens, Jean Golding, Cornelia M. van Duijn, John McLaughlin, Andrew Paterson, George Wells, Isabel Fortier, Matthew Freedman, Maja Zecevic, Richard A. King, Claire Infante-Rivard, M. Alexandre Stewart, and Nick Birkett
Reprinted with permission from PloS Medicine, Annals of Internal Medicine, European Journal of Clinical Investigation, European Journal of Epidemiology, Genetic Epidemiology, Human Genetics, and Journal of Clinical Epidemiology.
Introduction
The rapidly evolving evidence on genetic associations is crucial to integrating human genomics into the practice of medicine and public health (1,2). Genetic factors are likely to affect the occurrence of numerous common diseases, and therefore identifying and characterizing the associated risk (or protection) will be important in improving the understanding of etiology and potentially for developing interventions based on genetic information. The number of publications on the associations between genes and diseases has increased tremendously; with more than 34,000 published articles, the annual number has more than doubled between 2001 and 2008 (3,4). Articles on genetic associations have been published in about 1,500 journals and in several languages.
Despite the many similarities between genetic association studies and “classical” observational epidemiologic studies (i.e., cross-sectional, case-control, and cohort) of lifestyle and environmental factors, genetic association studies present several specific challenges including an unprecedented volume of new data (5,6) and the likelihood of very small individual effects. Genes may operate in complex pathways with gene–environment and gene–gene interactions (7). Moreover, the current evidence base on gene–disease associations is fraught with methodologic problems (8–10). Inadequate reporting of results, even from well-conducted studies, hampers assessment of a study’s strengths and weaknesses, and hence the integration of evidence (11).
Although several commentaries on the conduct, appraisal, and/or reporting of genetic association studies have so far been published (12–39), their recommendations differ. For example, some papers suggest that replication of findings should be part of the publication (12,13,16,17,23,26,34–36) whereas others consider this suggestion unnecessary or even unreasonable (21,40–44). In many publications, the guidance has focused on genetic association studies of specific diseases (14,15,17,19,22,23,25,26,31–38) or the design and conduct of genetic association studies (13–15,17,19,20,22,23,25,30–32,35,36) rather than on the quality of the reporting.
Despite increasing recognition of these problems, the quality of reporting genetic association studies needs to be improved (45–49). For example, an assessment of a random sample of 315 genetic association studies published from 2001 to 2003 found that most studies provided some qualitative descriptions of the study participants (e.g., origin and enrolment criteria), but reporting of quantitative descriptors, such as age and sex, was variable (49). In addition, completeness of reporting of methods that allow readers to assess potential biases (e.g., number of exclusions or number of samples that could not be genotyped) varied (49). Only some studies described methods to validate genotyping or mentioned whether research staff were blinded to outcome. The same problems persisted in a smaller sample of studies published in 2006 (49). Lack of transparency and incomplete reporting have raised concerns in a range of health research fields (11,50–53) and poor reporting has been associated with biased estimates of effects in clinical intervention studies (54).
The main goal of this article is to propose and justify a set of guiding principles for reporting results of genetic association studies. The epidemiology community has recently developed the STrengthening the Reporting of OBservational studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement for cross-sectional, case-control, and cohort studies (55,56). Given the relevance of general epidemiologic principles for genetic association studies, we propose recommendations in an extension of the STROBE Statement called the STrengthening the REporting of Genetic Association studies (STREGA) Statement. The recommendations of the STROBE Statement have a strong foundation because they are based on empirical evidence on the reporting of observational studies, and they involved extensive consultations in the epidemiologic research community (56). We have sought to identify gaps and areas of controversy in the evidence regarding potential biases in genetic association studies. With the recommendations, we have indicated available empirical or theoretical work that has demonstrated or suggested that a methodological feature of a study can influence the direction or magnitude of the association observed. We acknowledge that for many items, no such evidence exists. The intended audience for the reporting guideline is broad and includes epidemiologists, geneticists, statisticians, clinician scientists, and laboratory-based investigators who undertake genetic association studies. In addition, it includes “users” of such studies who wish to understand the basic premise, design, and limitations of genetic association studies in order to interpret the results. The field of genetic associations is evolving very rapidly with the advent of genome-wide association investigations, high-throughput platforms assessing genetic variability beyond common single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) (e.g., copy number variants, rare variants), and eventually routine full sequencing of samples from large populations. Our recommendations are not intended to support or oppose the choice of any particular study design or method. Instead, they are intended to maximize the transparency, quality, and completeness of reporting of what was done and found in a particular study.
Methods
A multidisciplinary group developed the STREGA Statement by using literature review, workshop presentations and discussion, and iterative electronic correspondence after the workshop. Thirty-three of 74 invitees participated in the STREGA workshop in Ottawa, Ontario, Canada, in June 2006. Participants included epidemiologists, geneticists, statisticians, journal editors, and graduate students.
Before the workshop, an electronic search was performed to identify existing reporting guidance for genetic association studies. Workshop participants were also asked to identify any additional guidance. They prepared brief presentations on existing reporting guidelines, empirical evidence on reporting of genetic association studies, the development of the STROBE Statement, and several key areas for discussion that were identified on the basis of consultations before the workshop. These areas included the selection and participation of study participants, rationale for choice of genes and variants investigated, genotyping errors, methods for inferring haplotypes, population stratification, assessment of Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE), multiple testing, reporting of quantitative (continuous) outcomes, selectively reporting study results, joint effects, and inference of causation in single studies. Additional resources to inform workshop participants were the HuGENet handbook (57,58), examples of data extraction forms from systematic reviews or meta-analyses, articles on guideline development (59,60), and the checklists developed for STROBE. To harmonize our recommendations for genetic association studies with those for observational epidemiologic studies, we communicated with the STROBE group during the development process and sought their comments on the STREGA draft documents. We also provided comments on the developing STROBE Statement and its associated explanation and elaboration document (56).
Results
In Table 10.1, we present the STREGA recommendations, an extension to the STROBE checklist (55) for genetic association studies. The resulting STREGA checklist provides additions to 12 of the 22 items on the STROBE checklist. During the workshop and subsequent consultations, we identified five main areas of special interest that are specific to, or especially relevant in, genetic association studies: genotyping errors, population stratification, modeling haplotype variation, HWE, and replication. We elaborate on each of these areas, starting each section with the corresponding STREGA recommendation, followed by a brief outline of the issue and an explanation for the recommendations. Complementary information on these areas and the rationale for additional STREGA recommendations relating to selection of participants, choice of genes and variants selected, treatment effects in studying quantitative traits, statistical methods, relatedness, reporting of descriptive and outcome data, and issues of data volume are presented in Table 10.2.
Genotyping Errors
Recommendation for reporting of methods (Table 10.1, item 8b): Describe laboratory methods, including source and storage of DNA, genotyping methods and platforms (including the allele calling algorithm used, and its version), error rates, and call rates. State the laboratory/center where genotyping was done. Describe comparability of laboratory methods if there is more than one group. Specify whether genotypes were assigned using all of the data from the study simultaneously or in smaller batches.
Recommendation for reporting of results (Table 10.1, item 13a): Report numbers of individuals in whom genotyping was attempted and numbers of individuals in whom genotyping was successful.
Genotyping errors can occur as a result of effects of the DNA sequence flanking the marker of interest, poor quality or quantity of the DNA extracted from biological samples, biochemical artifacts, poor equipment precision or equipment failure, or human error in sample handling, conduct of the array, or handling the data obtained from the array (61). A commentary published in 2005 on the possible causes and consequences of genotyping errors observed that an increasing number of researchers were aware of the problem, but that the effects of such errors had largely been neglected (61). The magnitude of genotyping errors has been reported to vary between 0.5% and 30% (61–64). In high-throughput centers, an error rate of 0.5% per genotype has been observed for blind duplicates that were run on the same gel (64). This lower error rate reflects an explicit choice of markers for which genotyping rates have been found to be highly repeatable and whose individual polymerase chain reactions (PCR) have been optimized. Nondifferential genotyping errors, that is, those that do not differ systematically according to outcome status, will usually bias associations toward the null (65,66), just as for other nondifferential errors. The most marked bias occurs when genotyping sensitivity is poor and genotype prevalence is high (>85%) or, as the corollary, when genotyping specificity is poor and genotype prevalence is low (<15%) (65). When measurement of the environmental exposure has substantial error, genotyping errors of the order of 3% can lead to substantial underestimation of the magnitude of an interaction effect (95). When there are systematic differences in genotyping according to outcome status (differential error), bias in any direction may occur. Unblinded assessment may lead to differential misclassification. For genome-wide association studies of SNPs, differential misclassification between comparison groups (e.g., cases and controls) can occur because of differences in DNA storage, collection, or processing protocols, even when the genotyping itself meets the highest possible standards (71). In this situation, using samples blinded to comparison group to determine the parameters for allele calling could still lead to differential misclassification. To minimize such differential misclassification, it would be necessary to calibrate the software separately for each group. This is one of the reasons for our recommendation to specify whether genotypes were assigned using all of the data from the study simultaneously or in smaller batches.
Population Stratification
Recommendation for reporting of methods (Table 10.1, item 12h): Describe any methods used to assess or address population stratification.
Population stratification is the presence within a population of subgroups among which allele (or genotype, or haplotype) frequencies and disease risks differ. When the groups compared in the study differ in their proportions of the population subgroups, an association between the genotype and the disease being investigated may reflect the genotype being an indicator identifying a population subgroup rather than a causal variant. In this situation, population subgroup is a confounder because it is associated with both genotype frequency and disease risk. The potential implications of population stratification for the validity of genetic association studies have been debated (96–110). Modeling the possible effect of population stratification (when no effort has been made to address it) suggests that the effect is likely to be small in most situations (102,103,105–107). Meta-analyses of 43 gene–disease associations comprising 697 individual studies showed consistent associations across groups of different ethnic origin (107), and thus provide evidence against a large effect of population stratification, hidden or otherwise. However, as studies of association and interaction typically address moderate or small effects and hence require large sample sizes, a small bias arising from population stratification may be important (108). Study design (case-family control studies) and statistical methods (75) have been proposed to address population stratification, but so far few studies have used these suggestions (49). Most of the early genome-wide association studies used family-based designs or such methods as genomic control and principal components analysis (69,111) to control for stratification. These approaches are particularly appropriate for addressing bias when the identified genetic effects are very small (odds ratio < 1.20), as has been the situation in many recent genome-wide association studies (69,76,112–129). In view of the debate about the potential implications of population stratification for the validity of genetic association studies, we recommend transparent reporting of the methods used, or stating that none was used, to address this potential problem. This reporting will enable empirical evidence to accrue about the effects of population stratification and methods to address it.
Modeling Haplotype Variation
Recommendation for reporting of methods (Table 10.1, item 12g): Describe any methods used for inferring genotypes or haplotypes.
A haplotype is a combination of specific alleles at neighboring genes that tend to be inherited together. There has been considerable interest in modeling haplotype variation within candidate genes. Typically, the number of haplotypes observed within a gene is much smaller than the theoretical number of all possible haplotypes (130,131). Motivation for utilizing haplotypes comes, in large part, from the fact that multiple SNPs may “tag” an untyped variant more effectively than a single typed variant. The subset of SNPs used in such an approach is called “haplotype tagging” SNPs. Implicitly, an aim of haplotype tagging is to reduce the number of SNPs that have to be genotyped, while maintaining statistical power to detect an association with the phenotype. Maps of human genetic variation are becoming more complete, and large-scale genotypic analysis is becoming increasingly feasible. In consequence, it is possible that modeling haplotype variation will become more focused on rare causal variants, because these may not be included in the genotyping platforms.
In most current, large-scale genetic association studies, data are collected as unphased multilocus genotypes (i.e., which alleles are aligned together on particular segments of chromosome is unknown). It is common in such studies to use statistical methods to estimate haplotypes (132–135), and their accuracy and efficiency have been discussed (136–140). Some methods attempt to make use of a concept called haplotype “blocks” (141,142), but the results of these methods are sensitive to the specific definitions of the “blocks” (143,144). Reporting of the methods used to infer individual haplotypes and population haplotype frequencies, along with their associated uncertainties should enhance our understanding of the possible effects of different methods of modeling haplotype variation on study results as well as enabling comparison and syntheses of results from different studies.
Information on common patterns of genetic variation revealed by the International Haplotype Map (HapMap) Project (131) can be applied in the analysis of genome-wide association studies to infer genotypic variation at markers not typed directly in these studies (145,146). Essentially, these methods perform haplotype-based tests but make use of information on variation in a set of reference samples (e.g., HapMap) to guide the specific tests of association, collapsing a potentially large number of haplotypes into two classes (the allelic variation) at each marker. It is expected that these techniques will increase power in individual studies, and will aid in combining data across studies, and even across differing genotyping platforms. If imputation procedures have been used, it is useful to know the method, accuracy thresholds for acceptable imputation, how imputed genotypes were handled or weighted in the analysis, and whether any associations based on imputed genotypes were also verified on the basis of direct genotyping at a subsequent stage.
Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium
Recommendation for reporting of methods (Table 10.1, item 12f): State whether Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium was considered and, if so, how.
Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium has become widely accepted as an underlying model in population genetics after Hardy (147) and Weinberg (148) proposed the concept that genotype frequencies at a genetic locus are stable within one generation of random mating; the assumption of HWE is equivalent to the independence of two alleles at a locus. Views differ on whether testing for departure from HWE is a useful method to detect errors or peculiarities in the data set, and also the method of testing (149). In particular, it has been suggested that deviation from HWE may be a sign of genotyping errors (73,150,151). Testing for departure from HWE has a role in detecting gross errors of genotyping in large-scale genotyping projects such as identifying SNPs for which the clustering algorithms used to call genotypes have broken down (69,70). However, the statistical power to detect less important errors of genotyping by testing for departure from HWE is low (68) and, in hypothetical data, the presence of HWE was generally not altered by the introduction of genotyping errors (78). Furthermore, the assumptions underlying HWE, including random mating, lack of selection according to genotype, and absence of mutation or gene flow, are rarely met in human populations (152,153). In 5 of 42 gene–disease associations assessed in meta-analyses of almost 600 studies, the results of studies that violated HWE significantly differed from results of studies that conformed to the model (154). Moreover, the study suggested that exclusion of HWE-violating studies may result in loss of the statistical significance of some postulated gene–disease associations and that adjustment for the magnitude of deviation from the model may also have the same consequence for some other gene–disease associations. Given the differing views about the value of testing for departure from HWE and about the test methods, transparent reporting of whether such testing was done and, if so, the method used, is important for allowing the empirical evidence to accrue.
For massive testing platforms, such as genome-wide association studies, it might be expected that many false positive violations of HWE would occur if a lenient p-value threshold were set. There is no consensus on the appropriate p-value threshold for HWE-related quality control in this setting. Hence, we recommend that investigators state which threshold they have used, if any, to exclude specific polymorphisms from further consideration. For SNPs with low minor allele frequencies, substantially more significant results than expected by chance have been observed, and the distribution of alleles at these loci has often been found to show departure from HWE.
For genome-wide association studies, another approach that has been used to detect errors or peculiarities in the data set (due to population stratification, genotyping error, HWE deviations, or other reasons) has been to construct quantile–quantile (Q/Q) plots whereby observed association statistics or calculated p-values for each SNP are ranked in order from smallest to largest and plotted against the expected null distribution (68,70). The shape of the curve can lend insight into whether or not systematic biases are present.
Replication
Recommendation: State if the study is the first report of a genetic association, a replication effort, or both (Table 10.1, item 3).
Articles that present and synthesize data from several studies in a single report are becoming more common. In particular, many genome-wide association analyses describe several different study populations, sometimes with different study designs and genotyping platforms, and in various stages of discovery and replication (68,70). When data from several studies are presented in a single original report, each of the constituent studies and the composite results should be fully described. For example, a discussion of sample size and the reason for arriving at that size would include clear differentiation between the initial group (those that were typed with the full set of SNPs) and those that were included in the replication phase only (typed with a reduced set of SNPs) (68,70). Describing the methods and results in sufficient detail would require substantial space in print, but options for publishing additional information on the study online make this possible.
Discussion
The choices made for study design, conduct, and data analysis potentially influence the magnitude and direction of results of genetic association studies. However, the empirical evidence on these effects is insufficient. Transparency of reporting is thus essential for developing a better evidence base (Table 10.2). Transparent reporting helps address gaps in empirical evidence (45), such as the effects of incomplete participation and genotyping errors. It will also help assess the impact of currently controversial issues such as population stratification, methods of inferring haplotypes, departure from HWE, and multiple testing on effect estimates under different study conditions.
The STREGA Statement proposes a minimum checklist of items for reporting genetic association studies. The statement has several strengths. First, it is based on existing guidance on reporting observational studies (STROBE). Second, it was developed from discussions of an interdisciplinary group that included epidemiologists, geneticists, statisticians, journal editors, and graduate students, thus reflecting a broad collaborative approach in terminology accessible to scientists from diverse disciplines. Finally, it explicitly describes the rationale for the decisions (Table 10.2) and has a clear plan for dissemination and evaluation.
The STREGA recommendations are available at http://www.strega-statement. org. We welcome comments, which will be used to refine future versions of the recommendations. We note that little is known about the most effective ways to apply reporting guidelines in practice, and that therefore it has been suggested that editors and authors collect, analyze, and report their experiences in using such guidelines (155). We consider that the STREGA recommendations can be used by authors, peer reviewers, and editors to improve the reporting of genetic association studies. We invite journals to endorse STREGA, for example by including STREGA and its Web address in their Instructions for Authors and by advising authors and peer reviewers to use the checklist as a guide. It has been suggested that reporting guidelines are most helpful if authors keep the general content of the guideline items in mind as they write their initial drafts, then refer to the details of individual items as they critically appraise what they have written during the revision process (155). We emphasize that the STREGA reporting guidelines should not be used for screening submitted manuscripts to determine the quality or validity of the study being reported. Adherence to the recommendations may make some manuscripts longer, and this may be seen as a drawback in an era of limited space in a print journal. However, the ability to post information on the Web should alleviate this concern. The place in which supplementary information is presented can be decided by authors and editors of the individual journal.
We hope that the recommendations stimulate transparent and improved reporting of genetic association studies. In turn, better reporting of original studies would facilitate the synthesis of available research results and the further development of study methods in genetic epidemiology with the ultimate goal of improving the understanding of the role of genetic factors in the cause of diseases.
Summary
Making sense of rapidly evolving evidence on genetic associations is crucial to making genuine advances in human genomics and the eventual integration of this information in the practice of medicine and public health. Assessment of the strengths and weaknesses of this evidence, and hence the ability to synthesize it, has been limited by inadequate reporting of results. The STrengthening the REporting of Genetic Association (STREGA) studies initiative builds on the STrengthening the Reporting of OBservational studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) STatement and provides additions to 12 of the 22 items on the STROBE checklist. The additions concern population stratification, genotyping errors, modeling haplotype variation, Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium, replication, selection of participants, rationale for choice of genes and variants, treatment effects in studying quantitative traits, statistical methods, relatedness, reporting of descriptive and outcome data, and the volume of data issues that are important to consider in genetic association studies. The STREGA recommendations do not prescribe or dictate how a genetic association study should be designed but seek to enhance the transparency of its reporting, regardless of choices made during design, conduct, or analysis.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Kyle Vogan and Allen Wilcox for their participation in the workshop and for their comments; Michele Cargill (Affymetrix, Inc.) and Aaron del Duca (DNA Genotek) for their participation in the workshop as observers; and the Public Population Project in Genomics (P3G), hosted by the University of Montreal and supported by Genome Canada and Genome Quebec. This article was made possible thanks to input and discussion by the P3G International Working Group on Epidemiology and Biostatistics, discussion held in Montreal, May 2007. The authors also thank the reviewers for their very thoughtful feedback, and Silvia Visentin, Rob Moriarity, Morgan Macneill, and Valery L’Heureux for administrative support. We were unable to contact Barbara Cohen to confirm her involvement in the latest version of this chapter.
Tables
References
- Khoury MJ, Little J, Burke W. Human genome epidemiology: scope and strategies. In: Khoury MJ, Little J, Burke W, eds. Human Genome Epidemiology: A Scientific Foundation for Using Genetic Information to Improve Health and Prevent Disease. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004:3–16.
- Genomics, Health and Society Working Group. Genomics, Health and Society. Emerging Issues for Public Policy. Ottawa: Government of Canada Policy Research Initiative; 2004.
- Lin BK, Clyne M, Walsh M, et al. Tracking the epidemiology of human genes in the literature: the HuGE published literature database. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;164:1–4.
- Yu W, Yesupriya A, Clyne M, et al. HuGE Literature Finder. HuGE Navigator.
- Lawrence RW, Evans DM, Cardon LR. Prospects and pitfalls in whole genome association studies. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005;360:1589–1595.
- Thomas DC. Are we ready for genome-wide association studies? Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2006;15:595–598.
- Khoury MJ, Little J, Gwinn M, et al. On the synthesis and interpretation of consistent but weak gene–disease associations in the era of genome-wide association studies. Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36:439–445.
- Little J, Khoury MJ, Bradley L, et al. The human genome project is complete. How do we develop a handle for the pump? Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157:667–673.
- Ioannidis JP, Bernstein J, Boffetta P, et al. A network of investigator networks in human genome epidemiology. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;162:302–304.
- Ioannidis JP, Gwinn M, Little J, et al. A road map for efficient and reliable human genome epidemiology. Nat Genet. 2006;38:3–5.
- von Elm E, Egger M. The scandal of poor epidemiological research. BMJ. 2004;329:868–869.
- Anonymous. Freely associating (editorial). Nat Genet. 1999;22:1–2.
- Cardon L, Bell J. Association study designs for complex diseases. Nat Rev Genet. 2001;2:91–99.
- Weiss S. Association studies in asthma genetics. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2001;164:2014–2015.
- Weiss ST, Silverman EK, Palmer LJ. Case-control association studies in pharmacogenetics. Pharmacogenomics. 2001;J1:157–158.
- Cooper DN, Nussbaum RL, Krawczak M. Proposed guidelines for papers describing DNA polymorphism-disease associations. Hum Genet. 2002;110:208.
- Hegele R. SNP judgements and freedom of association. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 2002;22:1058–1061.
- Little J, Bradley L, Bray MS, et al. Reporting, appraising, and integrating data on genotype prevalence and gene–disease associations. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;156:300–310.
- Romero R, Kuivaniemi H, Tromp G, et al. The design, execution, and interpretation of genetic association studies to decipher complex diseases. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2002;187:1299–1312.
- Colhoun HM, McKeigue PM, Davey Smith G. Problems of reporting genetic associations with complex outcomes. Lancet. 2003;361:865–872.
- van Duijn CM, Porta M. Good prospects for genetic and molecular epidemiologic studies in the European Journal of Epidemiology. Eur J Epidemiol 2003;18:285–286.
- Crossman D, Watkins H. Jesting Pilate, genetic case-control association studies, and Heart. Heart. 2004;90:831–832.
- Huizinga TW, Pisetsky DS, Kimberly RP. Associations, populations, and the truth: recommendations for genetic association studies in arthritis & rheumatism. Arthritis Rheum. 2004;50:2066–2071.
- Little J. Reporting and review of human genome epidemiology studies. In: Khoury MJ, Little J, Burke W, eds. Human Genome Epidemiology: A Scientific Foundation for Using Genetic Information to Improve Health and Prevent Disease. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004:168–192.
- Rebbeck TR, Martinez ME, Sellers TA, et al. Genetic variation and cancer: improving the environment for publication of association studies. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2004;13:1985–1986.
- Tan N, Mulley J, Berkovic S. Association studies in epilepsy: “The truth is out there.” Epilepsia. 2004;45:1429–1442.
- Anonymous. Framework for a fully powered risk engine. Nat Genet. 2005;37:1153.
- Ehm MG, Nelson MR, Spurr NK. Guidelines for conducting and reporting whole genome/large-scale association studies. Hum Mol Genet. 2005;14:2485–2488.
- Freimer NB, Sabatti C. Guidelines for association studies in human molecular genetics. Hum Mol Genet. 2005;14:2481–2483.
- Hattersley AT, McCarthy MI. What makes a good genetic association study? Lancet. 2005;366:1315–1323.
- Manly K. Reliability of statistical associations between genes and disease. Immunogenetics. 2005;57:549–558.
- Shen H, Liu Y, Liu P, et al. Nonreplication in genetic studies of complex diseases— lessons learned from studies of osteoporosis and tentative remedies. J Bone Miner Res. 2005;20:365–376.
- Vitali S, Randolph A. Assessing the quality of case-control association studies on the genetic basis of sepsis. Pediatr Crit Care Med. 2005;6:S74–S77.
- Wedzicha JA, Hall IP. Publishing genetic association studies in Thorax. Thorax. 2005;60:357.
- Hall IP, Blakey JD. Genetic association studies in Thorax. Thorax. 2005;60:357–359.
- DeLisi LE, Faraone SV. When is a “positive” association truly a “positive” in psychiatric genetics? A commentary based on issues debated at the World Congress of Psychiatric Genetics, Boston, October 12–18, 2005. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2006;141:319–322.
- Saito YA, Talley NJ, de Andrade M, et al. Case-control genetic association studies in gastrointestinal disease: review and recommendations. Am J Gastroenterol. 2006;101:1379–1389.
- Uhlig K, Menon V, Schmid CH. Recommendations for reporting of clinical research studies. Am J Kidney Dis. 2007;49:3–7.
- NCI-NHGRI Working Group on Replication in Association Studies, Chanock SJ, Manolio T, Boehnke M, et al. Replicating genotype-phenotype associations. Nature. 2007;447:655–660.
- Begg CB. Reflections on publication criteria for genetic association studies. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2005;14:1364–1365.
- Byrnes G, Gurrin L, Dowty J, et al. Publication policy or publication bias? Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2005;14:1363.
- Pharoah PD, Dunning AM, Ponder BA, et al. The reliable identification of disease-gene associations. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2005;14:1362.
- Wacholder S. Publication environment and broad investigation of the genome. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2005;14:1361.
- Whittemore AS. Genetic association studies: time for a new paradigm? Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2005;14:1359–1360.
- Bogardus ST, Jr, Concato J, Feinstein AR. Clinical epidemiological quality in molecular genetic research. The need for methodological standards. JAMA. 1999;281:1919–1926.
- Peters DL, Barber RC, Flood EM, et al. Methodologic quality and genotyping reproducibility in studies of tumor necrosis factor -308 G→A single nucleotide polymorphism and bacterial sepsis: implications for studies of complex traits. Crit Care Med. 2003;31:1691–1696.
- Clark MF, Baudouin SV. A systematic review of the quality of genetic association studies in human sepsis. Intensive Care Med. 2006;32:1706–1712.
- Lee W, Bindman J, Ford T, et al. Bias in psychiatric case-control studies: literature survey. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190:204–209.
- Yesupriya A, Evangelou E, Kavvoura FK, et al. Reporting of human genome epidemiology (HuGE) association studies: an empirical assessment. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2008;8:31.
- Reid MC, Lachs MS, Feinstein AR. Use of methodological standards in diagnostic test research. Getting better but still not good. JAMA. 1995;274:645–651.
- Brazma A, Hingamp P, Quackenbush J, et al. Minimum information about a microarray experiment (MIAME)—toward standards for microarray data. Nat Genet. 2001;29:356–371.
- Pocock SJ, Collier TJ, Dandreo KJ, et al. Issues in the reporting of epidemiological studies: a survey of recent practice. BMJ. 2004;329:883.
- Altman D, Moher D. Developing guidelines for reporting healthcare research: scientific rationale and procedures. Med Clin (Barc). 2005;125:8–13.
- Gluud LL. Bias in clinical intervention research. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;163:493–501.
- von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, et al. The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: Guidelines for reporting observational studies. PLoS Med. 2007;4:e296. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0040296.
- Vandenbroucke JP, von Elm E, Altman DG, et al. Strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE): explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med. 2007;147:W163–W194.
- Little J, Higgins JPT, eds. The HuGENet™ HuGE Review Handbook, version 1.0.
- Higgins JP, Little J, Ioannidis JP, et al. Turning the pump handle: evolving methods for integrating the evidence on gene–disease association. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;166:863–866.
- Altman DG, Schulz KF, Moher D, et al. The revised CONSORT statement for reporting randomized trials: explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med. 2001;134:663–694.
- Moher D, Schultz KF, Altman D. The CONSORT statement: revised recommendations for improving the quality of reports of parallelgroup randomized trials. JAMA. 2001;285:1987–1991.
- Pompanon F, Bonin A, Bellemain E, et al. Genotyping errors: causes, consequences and solutions. Nat Rev Genet. 2005;6:847–859.
- Akey JM, Zhang K, Xiong M, et al. The effect that genotyping errors have on the robustness of common linkage disequilibrium measures. Am J Hum Genet. 2001;68:1447–1456.
- Dequeker E, Ramsden S, Grody WW, et al. Quality control in molecular genetic testing. Nat Rev Genet. 2001;2:717–723.
- Mitchell AA, Cutler DJ, Chakravarti A. Undetected genotyping errors cause apparent overtransmission of common alleles in the transmission/disequilibrium test. Am J Hum Genet. 2003;72:598–610.
- Rothman N, Stewart WF, Caporaso NE, et al. Misclassification of genetic susceptibility biomarkers: implications for case-control studies and cross-population comparisons. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 1993;2:299–303.
- Garcia-Closas M, Wacholder S, Caporaso N, et al. Inference issues in cohort and case-control studies of genetic effects and gene environment interactions. In: Khoury MJ, Little J, Burke W, eds. Human Genome Epidemiology: A Scientific Foundation for Using Genetic Information to Improve Health and Prevent Disease. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004:127–144.
- Steinberg K, Gallagher M. Assessing genotypes in human genome epidemiology studies. In: Khoury MJ, Little J, Burke W, eds. Human Genome Epidemiology: A Scientific Foundation for Using Genetic Information to Improve Health and Prevent Disease. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004:79–91.
- McCarthy MI, Abecasis GR, Cardon LR, et al. Genome-wide association studies for complex traits: consensus, uncertainty and challenges. Nat Rev Genet. 2008;9:356–369.
- Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium. Genome-wide association study of 14,000 cases of seven common diseases and 3,000 shared controls. Nature. 2007;447:661–678.
- Pearson TA, Manolio TA. How to interpret a genome-wide association study. JAMA. 2008;299:1335–1344.
- Clayton DG, Walker NM, Smyth DJ, et al. Population structure, differential bias and genomic control in a large-scale, case-control association study. Nat Genet. 2005;37:1243–1246.
- Plagnol V, Cooper JD, Todd JA, et al. A method to address differential bias in genotyping in large-scale association studies. PLoS Genet. 2007;3:e74. doi:10.1371/journal. pgen.0030074.
- Salanti G, Amountza G, Ntzani EE, et al. Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium in genetic association studies: an empirical evaluation of reporting, deviations, and power. Eur J Hum Genet. 2005;13:840–848.
- Winker MA. Race and ethnicity in medical research: requirements meet reality. J Law Med Ethics. 2006;34:520–525, 480.
- Balding DJ. A tutorial on statistical methods for population association studies. Nat Rev Genet. 2006;7:781–791.
- Scott LJ, Mohlke KL, Bonnycastle LL, et al. A genome-wide association study of type 2 diabetes in Finns detects multiple susceptibility variants. Science. 2007;316:1341–1345.
- Scuteri A, Sanna S, Chen WM, et al. Genome-wide association scan shows genetic variants in the FTO gene are associated with obesity-related traits. PLoS Genet. 2007;3:e115. doi:10.1371/journal. pgen.0030115.
- Zou GY, Donner A. The merits of testing Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium in the analysis of unmatched case-control data: a cautionary note. Ann Hum Genet. 2006;70:923–933.
- Chan AW, Hrobjartsson A, Haahr MT, et al. Empirical evidence for selective reporting of outcomes in randomized trials: comparison of protocols to published articles. JAMA. 2004;291:2457–2465.
- Chan AW, Krleza-Jeric K, Schmid I, et al. Outcome reporting bias in randomized trials funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. CMAJ. 2004;171:735–740.
- Chan AW, Altman DG. Identifying outcome reporting bias in randomised trials on PubMed: review of publications and survey of authors. BMJ. 2005;330:753.
- Contopoulos-Ioannidis DG, Alexiou GA, Gouvias TC, et al. An empirical evaluation of multifarious outcomes in pharmacogenetics: beta-2 adrenoceptor gene polymorphisms in asthma treatment. Pharmacogenet Genomics. 2006;16:705–711.
- Wain HM, Bruford EA, Lovering RC, et al. Guidelines for human gene nomenclature. Genomics. 2002;79:464–470.
- Wain HM, Lush M, Ducluzeau F, et al. Genew: the human gene nomenclature database. Nucleic Acids Res. 2002;30:169–171.
- Sherry ST, Ward MH, Kholodov M, et al. dbSNP: the NCBI database of genetic variation. Nucleic Acids Res. 2001;29:308–311.
- Antonarakis SE. Recommendations for a nomenclature system for human gene mutations. Nomenclature working group. Hum Mutat. 1998;11:1–3.
- den Dunnen JT, Antonarakis SE. Mutation nomenclature extensions and suggestions to describe complex mutations: a discussion. Hum Mutat. 2000;15:7–12.
- Tobin MD, Sheehan NA, Scurrah KJ, et al. Adjusting for treatment effects in studies of quantitative traits: antihypertensive therapy and systolic blood pressure. Stat Med. 2005;24:2911–2935.
- Lynch M, Ritland K. Estimation of pairwise relatedness with molecular markers. Genetics. 1999;152:1753–1766.
- Slager SL, Schaid DJ. Evaluation of candidate genes in case-control studies: a statistical method to account for related subjects. Am J Hum Genet.2001;68:1457–1462.
- Voight BF, Pritchard JK. Confounding from cryptic relatedness in case-control association studies. PLoS Genet. 2005;1:e32. doi:10.1371/journal. pgen.0010032.
- Homer N, Szelinger S, Redman M, et al. Resolving individuals contributing trace amounts of DNA to highly complex mixtures using high-density SNP genotyping microarrays. PLoS Genet. 2008;4:e1000167. doi:10.1371/journal. pgen.1000167.
- Zerhouni EA, Nabel EG. Protecting aggregate genomic data. Science. 2008;322:44.
- Little J, Higgins JPT, Ioannidis JPA, et al. STrengthening the REporting of Genetic Association Studies (STREGA)—an extension of the STROBE statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6(2):e1000022.
- Wong MY, Day NE, Luan JA, et al. Estimation of magnitude in gene–environment interactions in the presence of measurement error. Stat Med. 2004;23:987–998.
- Knowler WC, Williams RC, Pettitt DJ, et al. Gm3;5,13,14 and type 2 diabetes mellitus: an association in American Indians with genetic admixture. Am J Human Genet. 1988; 43:520–526.
- Gelernter J, Goldman D, Risch N. The A1 allele at the D2 dopamine receptor gene and alcoholism: a reappraisal. JAMA. 1993;269:1673–1677.
- Kittles RA, Chen W, Panguluri RK, et al. CYP3A4-V and prostate cancer in African Americans: causal or confounding association because of population stratification? Hum Genet. 2002;110:553–560.
- Thomas DC, Witte JS. Point: population stratification: a problem for case control studies of candidate-gene associations? Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2002;11:505–512.
- Wacholder S, Chatterjee N, Hartge P. Joint effects of genes and environment distorted by selection biases: implications for hospital-based case-control studies. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2002;11:885–889.
- Cardon LR, Palmer LJ. Population stratification and spurious allelic association. Lancet. 2003;361:598–604.
- Wacholder S, Rothman N, Caporaso N. Population stratification in epidemiologic studies of common genetic variants and cancer: quantification of bias. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2000; 92:1151–1158.
- Ardlie KG, Lunetta KL, Seielstad M. Testing for population subdivision and association in four case-control studies. Am J Human Genet. 2002;71:304–311.
- Edland SD, Slager S, Farrer M. Genetic association studies in Alzheimer’s disease research: challenges and opportunities. Stat Med. 2004;23:169–178.
- Millikan RC. Re: population stratification in epidemiologic studies of common genetic variants and cancer: quantification of bias. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2001;93:156–157.
- Wang Y, Localio R, Rebbeck TR. Evaluating bias due to population stratification in case-control association studies of admixed populations. Genet Epidemiol. 2004;27:14–20.
- Ioannidis JP, Ntzani EE, Trikalinos TA. “Racial” differences in genetic effects for complex diseases. Nat Genet. 2004;36:1312–1318.
- Marchini J, Cardon LR, Phillips MS, et al. The effects of human population structure on large genetic association studies. Nat Genet. 2004;36:512–517.
- Freedman ML, Reich D, Penney KL, et al. Assessing the impact of population stratification on genetic association studies. Nat Genet. 2004;36:388–393.
- Khlat M, Cazes MH, Genin E, et al. Robustness of case-control studies of genetic factors to population stratification: magnitude of bias and type I error. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2004;13:1660–1664.
- Ioannidis JP. Non-replication and inconsistency in the genome-wide association setting. Hum Hered. 2007;64:203–213.
- Parkes M, Barrett JC, Prescott NJ, et al. Sequence variants in the autophagy gene IRGM and multiple other replicating loci contribute to Crohn’s disease susceptibility. Nat Genet. 2007;39:830–832.
- Todd JA, Walker NM, Cooper JD, et al. Robust associations of four new chromosome regions from genome-wide analyses of type 1 diabetes. Nat Genet. 2007;39:857–864.
- Zeggini E, Weedon MN, Lindgren CM, et al. Replication of genome-wide association signals in UK samples reveals risk loci for type 2 diabetes. Science. 2007;316:1336–1341.
- Diabetes Genetics Initiative of Broad Institute of Harvard and MIT, Lund University, and Novartis Institutes of BioMedical Research, Saxena R, Voight BF, et al. Genome-wide association analysis identifies loci for type 2 diabetes and triglyceride levels. Science. 2007;316:1331–1336.
- Helgadottir A, Thorleifsson G, Manolescu A, et al. A common variant on chromosome 9p21 affects the risk of myocardial infarction. Science. 2007;316:1491–1493.
- McPherson R, Pertsemlidis A, Kavaslar N, et al. A common allele on chromosome 9 associated with coronary heart disease. Science. 2007;316:1488–1491.
- Easton DF, Pooley KA, Dunning AM, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies novel breast cancer susceptibility loci. Nature. 2007;447:1087–1093.
- Hunter DJ, Kraft P, Jacobs KB, Cox DG, et al. A genome-wide association study identifies alleles in FGFR2 associated with risk of sporadic postmenopausal breast cancer. Nat Genet. 2007;39:870–874.
- Stacey SN, Manolescu A, Sulem P, et al. Common variants on chromosomes 2q35 and 16q12 confer susceptibility to estrogen receptor-positive breast cancer. Nat Genet. 2007;39:865–869.
- Gudmundsson J, Sulem P, Steinthorsdottir V, et al. Two variants on chromosome 17 confer prostate cancer risk, and the one in TCF2 protects against type 2 diabetes. Nat Genet. 2007;39:977–983.
- Haiman CA, Patterson N, Freedman ML, et al. Multiple regions within 8q24 independently affect risk for prostate cancer. Nat Genet. 2007;39:638–644.
- Yeager M, Orr N, Hayes RB, et al. Genome-wide association study of prostate cancer identifies a second risk locus at 8q24. Nat Genet. 2007;39:645–649.
- Zanke BW, Greenwood CM, Rangrej J, et al. Genome-wide association scan identifies a colorectal cancer susceptibility locus on chromosome 8q24. Nat Genet. 2007;39:989–994.
- Tomlinson I, Webb E, Carvajal-Carmona L, et al. A genome-wide association scan of tag SNPs identifies a susceptibility variant for colorectal cancer at 8q24.21. Nat Genet. 2007;39:984–988.
- Haiman CA, Le Marchand L, Yamamoto J, et al. A common genetic risk factor for colorectal and prostate cancer. Nat Genet. 2007;39:954–956.
- Rioux JD, Xavier RJ, Taylor KD, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies new susceptibility loci for Crohn disease and implicates autophagy in disease pathogenesis. Nat Genet. 2007;39:596–604.
- Libioulle C, Louis E, Hansoul S, et al. Novel Crohn disease locus identified by genome-wide association maps to a gene desert on 5p13.1 and modulates expression of PTGER4. PLoS Genet. 2007;3:e58. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.0030058.
- Duerr RH, Taylor KD, Brant SR, et al. A genome-wide association study identifies IL23R as an inflammatory bowel disease gene. Science. 2006;314:1461–1463.
- Zhao LP, Li SS, Khalid N. A method for the assessment of disease associations with single-nucleotide polymorphism haplotypes and environmental variables in case-control studies. Am J Hum Genet. 2003;72:1231–1250.
- International HapMap Consortium, Frazer KA, Ballinger DG, et al. A second generation human haplotype map of over 3.1 million SNPs. Nature. 2007;449:851–861.
- Stephens M, Smith NJ, Donnelly P. A new statistical method for haplotype reconstruction from population data. Am J Hum Genet. 2001;68:978–989.
- Qin ZS, Niu T, Liu JS. Partition-ligation-expectation-maximization algorithm for haplotype inference with single-nucleotide polymorphisms. Am J Hum Genet. 2002;71:1242–1247.
- Scheet P, Stephens M. A fast and flexible statistical model for large-scale population genotype data: applications to inferring missing genotypes and haplotypic phase. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78:629–644.
- Browning SR. Missing data imputation and haplotype phase inference for genome-wide association studies. Hum Genet. 2008;124:439–450.
- Huang Q, Fu YX, Boerwinkle E. Comparison of strategies for selecting single nucleotide polymorphisms for case/control association studies. Hum Genet. 2003;113:253–257.
- Kamatani N, Sekine A, Kitamoto T, et al. Largescale single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) and haplotype analyses, using dense SNP maps, of 199 drug-related genes in 752 subjects: the analysis of the association between uncommon SNPs within haplotype blocks and the haplotypes constructed with haplotype-tagging SNPs. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;75:190–203.
- Zhang W, Collins A, Morton NE. Does haplotype diversity predict power for association mapping of disease susceptibility? Hum Genet. 2004;115:157–164.
- Carlson CS, Eberle MA, Rieder MJ, et al. Selecting a maximally informative set of single-nucleotide polymorphisms for association analysis using linkage disequilibrium. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;74:106–120.
- van Hylckama Vlieg A, Sandkuijl LA, Rosendaal FR, et al. Candidate gene approach in association studies: Would the factor V leiden mutation have been found by this approach? Eur J Hum Genet. 2004;12:478–482.
- Greenspan G, Geiger D. Model-based inference of haplotype block variation. J Comput Biol. 2004;11:493–504.
- Kimmel G, Shamir R. GERBIL: Genotype resolution and block identification using likelihood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102:158–162.
- Cardon LR, Abecasis GR. Using haplotype blocks to map human complex trait loci. Trends Genet. 2003;19:135–140.
- Ke X, Hunt S, Tapper W, et al. The impact of SNP density on fine-scale patterns of linkage disequilibrium. Hum Mol Genet. 2004;13:577–588.
- Servin B, Stephens M. Imputation-based analysis of association studies: candidate regions and quantitative traits. PLoS Genet. 2007;3:e114. doi:10.1371/journal. pgen.0030114.
- Marchini J, Howie B, Myers S, et al. A new multipoint method for genome-wide association studies by imputation of genotypes. Nat Genet. 2007;39:906–913.
- Hardy GH. Mendelian proportions in a mixed population. Science. 1908;28:49–50.
- Weinberg W. Über den Nachweis der Vererbung beim Menschen. Jahrhefte Des Vereines Für Vaterländische Naturkunde in Württemberg. 1908;64:368–382.
- Minelli C, Thompson JR, Abrams KR, et al. How should we use information about HWE in the meta-analyses of genetic association studies? Int J Epidemiol. 2008;37:136–146.
- Xu J, Turner A, Little J, et al. Positive results in association studies are associated with departure from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium: hint for genotyping error? Hum Genet. 2002;111:573–574.
- Hosking L, Lumsden S, Lewis K, et al. Detection of genotyping errors by Hardy– Weinberg equilibrium testing. Eur J Hum Genet. 2004;12:395–399.
- Shoemaker J, Painter I, Weir BS. A Bayesian characterization of Hardy–Weinberg disequilibrium. Genetics. 1998;149:2079–2088.
- Ayres KL, Balding DJ. Measuring departures from Hardy–Weinberg: a Markov chain Monte Carlo method for estimating the inbreeding coefficient. Heredity. 1998;80:769–777.
- Trikalinos TA, Salanti G, Khoury MJ, et al. Impact of violations and deviations in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium on postulated gene–disease associations. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;163:300–309.
- Davidoff F, Batalden P, Stevens D, et al. Publication guidelines for improvement studies in health care: evolution of the SQUIRE project. Ann Intern Med. 2008;149:670–676.